CHANGES IN PAIN SENSITIVITY FOLLOWING SPINAL MANIPULATION: A SYSTEMATIC REVIEW AND META-ANALYSIS
 
   

Changes in Pain Sensitivity Following
Spinal Manipulation: A Systematic
Review and Meta-analysis

This section is compiled by Frank M. Painter, D.C.
Send all comments or additions to:
   Frankp@chiro.org
 
   

FROM: J Electromyogr Kinesiol. 2012 (Oct); 22 (5): 752–767 ~ FULL TEXT

  OPEN ACCESS   


Rogelio A Coronado, PT, CSCS, FAAOMPT, Charles W. Gay, DC,
Joel E. Bialosky, PT, PhD, FAAOMPT, Giselle D. Carnaby, PhD, MPH, SLP/CCC,
Mark D. Bishop, PT, PhD, and Steven Z. George, PT, PhD

College of Public Health and Health Professions,
University of Florida,
Gainesville, FL, United States.


Spinal manipulation (SMT) is commonly used for treating individuals experiencing musculoskeletal pain. The mechanisms of SMT remain unclear; however, pain sensitivity testing may provide insight into these mechanisms. The purpose of this systematic review is to examine the literature on the hypoalgesic effects of SMT on pain sensitivity measures and to quantify these effects using meta-analysis. We performed a systematic search of articles using CINAHL, MEDLINE, PsycINFO, and SPORTDiscus from each databases' inception until May 2011. We examined methodological quality of each study and generated pooled effect size estimates using meta-analysis software. Of 997 articles identified, 20 met inclusion criteria for this review. Pain sensitivity testing used in these studies included chemical, electrical, mechanical, and thermal stimuli applied to various anatomical locations. Meta-analysis was appropriate for studies examining the immediate effect of SMT on mechanical pressure pain threshold (PPT). Spinal manipulation (SMT) demonstrated a favorable effect over other interventions on increasing PPT. Subgroup analysis showed a significant effect of SMT on increasing mechanical pressure pain threshold (PPT) at the remote sites of stimulus application supporting a potential central nervous system mechanism. Future studies of SMT related hypoalgesia should include multiple experimental stimuli and test at multiple anatomical sites.



From the Full-Text Article:

INTRODUCTION

In the United States, spinal manipulation (SMT) is a commonly used intervention for the treatment of individuals experiencing pain [Nahin et al, 2009]. SMT is effective for some individuals experiencing musculoskeletal pain [Childs et al, 2004]. However, despite the clinical effectiveness, the mechanisms by which SMT reduces pain and disability remain largely unknown. Mechanistic research on SMT suggests that biomechanical and neurophysiological changes occur with the application of SMT [Bialosky et al, 2009a; Evans, 2002; Pickar, 2002; Vernon, 2000; Wright, 1995]. Studies using pain sensitivity testing for measuring responses to SMT are appropriate in considering potential mechanisms of SMT.

Reductions in pain sensitivity, or hypoalgesia, following SMT may be indicative of a mechanism related to the modulation of afferent input or central nervous system processing of pain. Characterizing this mechanism may provide some insight into how SMT produces clinical benefit [Staahl et al, 2009a; Staahl et al, 2009b]. For example, Bialosky et al [2009b] reported an immediate hypoalgesic response to a specific noxious thermal stimulus (temporal summation of pain) and not other noxious thermal stimuli following lumbar SMT in patients with low back pain. In this study, the reduction in pain sensitivity was observed in the lower extremity and not the upper extremity. The authors theorized the observed effect related to modulation of pain primarily at the level of the spinal cord since 1.) these changes were seen within lumbar innervated areas and not cervical innervated areas and 2.) the findings were specific to a measure of pain sensitivity (temporal summation of pain), and not other measures of pain sensitivity, suggesting an effect related to attenuation of dorsal horn excitability and not a generalized change in pain sensitivity.

The example illustrated above highlights principal information related to the methodology of pain sensitivity testing, especially in terms of elucidating potential mechanisms of SMT. The characteristics of pain sensitivity measures include the sensory modality used, the psychophysical response, and the location of stimulus application [Arendt-Nielsen et al, 2009; Staahl et al, 2004]. Pain sensitivity is measured through the application of different sensory modalities, such as thermal, mechanical, electrical, ischemic and chemical stimuli, to different tissues of the body such as skin, muscle, and viscera [Arendt-Nielsen et al, 2009; Staahl et al, 2004]. The psychophysical response to a quantifiable amount of stimulus is assessed by methods such as the minimal amount of stimulus to generate pain (threshold), or the change in pain sensitivity to repeated stimulation (temporal summation) or multiple locations of stimulation (spatial summation) [Arendt-Nielsen et al, 2009; Staahl et al, 2004]. The location of stimulus can be measured at regions local or remote to the injured area or area where the intervention will be applied. In mechanistic studies of SMT, pain sensitivity may be assessed before and immediately following an intervention to assess the immediate effects [Bialosky et al, 2009b; Fernandez-Carnero et al, 2008; Fernandez-de-las-Penas et al, 2007; George et al, 2006], or throughout a course of treatment to assess the relationship to clinical outcomes [Valencia et al, 2011; Werner et al, 2010].

Vernon [2000] previously conducted a qualitative review of studies investigating SMT-induced hypoalgesia and noted few articles investigating the hypoalgesic effects of SMT. The review proposed several objectives for future investigations including, but not limited to: 1.) identifying where in the CNS pain modulation is occurring, 2.) identifying the neurochemical mechanisms involved in pain modulation, 3.) investigating the cumulative effects of SMT, and 4.) elucidating if certain elements of the SMT procedure such as location and cavitation are directly related to hypoalgesia [Vernon, 2000]. In that review, a systematic appraisal of study quality was not conducted, nor was a pooled effect size estimate generated for a specific pain sensitivity measure such as pressure pain threshold. The latter may not have been possible at the time due to few studies utilizing similar pain sensitivity measures. These two factors are important because the quality of the studies helps in the interpretation of the findings and a pooled estimate from multiple studies could provide a more valid indicator of the effect size for SMT on pain sensitivity.

The purpose of this systematic review and meta-analysis was to synthesize the growing literature on the relationship between SMT and pain sensitivity and examine the hypoalgesic effect of SMT. Specifically, we were interested in the changes in pain sensitivity following SMT. Further, we hoped to assess whether the observed effect of SMT differed based on sample population or location of assessment. Studies of pain sensitivity in response to SMT have included both healthy [Bishop et al, 2011; Fernandez-de-las-Penas et al, 2007; George et al, 2006] and clinical samples [Bialosky et al, 2009b; Fernandez-Carnero et al, 2008; Vernon et al, 1990]. Differences in pain sensitivity responses to SMT may exist considering chronic pain states are associated with altered pain sensitivity [Blumenstiel et al, 2011; Chua et al, 2011; Staud, 2010; Wallin et al, 2011]. For example, chronic low back pain is associated with generalized enhanced pressure pain sensitivity as compared to individuals without low back pain [Giesecke et al, 2004; O’Neill et al, 2007]. Therefore, we were interested in whether any observed changes differed by population (clinical vs. healthy). Finally, we were interested in whether SMT related changes in pain sensitivity differed by the location of the stimulus assessment (local to SMT application vs. remote to SMT application). Changes in pain sensitivity at the site of application of SMT, but not at remote regions, would indicate the effects of SMT are specific to the location of application. On the other hand, remote changes may be indicative of a more general effect, one mediated through modulation of nociceptive afferent processing within the central nervous system.



METHODS

This review was conducted in accordance with guidelines from Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic reviews and Meta-Analysis (PRISMA) and the Cochrane Back Review Group [Furlan et al, 2009; Liberati et al, 2009].

      Eligibility Criteria

Study Type   We included published randomized-controlled trials (RCT) that investigated the effects of SMT on pain sensitivity. Designs could include parallel (2 or more groups) or crossover (1 group) trials. We excluded case reports, case series, and single-case study designs.

Participants   The sample populations of interest were asymptomatic (e.g. healthy) and symptomatic (e.g. reporting a current musculoskeletal pain complaint) human participants of any age or sex. We did not limit the sample to participants with a specific clinical condition, as SMT is applied to those with various musculoskeletal conditions including extremity disorders [Iverson et al, 2008; Mintken et al, 2010]. We excluded studies investigating the effect of SMT on non-musculoskeletal conditions (e.g. asthma).

Intervention   The intervention of interest was SMT and we operationally defined this as a high-velocity, low-amplitude thrust technique targeted to a spinal region that may or may not result in an audible cavitation of a joint(s). Other synonymous terms for SMT used in studies include grade V mobilization, thrust mobilization/manipulation, or spinal adjustment. The SMT technique could be applied by the practitioner’s hand or with an instrument. The technique(s) could be provided multiple times to the same spinal region or to various spinal regions during a single session or over multiple sessions. Co-interventions could also be included within the treatment session if these same co-interventions were implemented in the comparison group. This allows for differences in treatment effect to be attributed to the addition of SMT in the experimental group. Conversely, we excluded studies in which SMT was provided with multiple co-interventions where the exclusive effect of SMT could not be established. For example, we excluded studies using multi-modal treatments (exercise + SMT + medication) compared to other forms of management.

Comparison   The comparison group could include any form of active or non-active intervention. Active interventions included exercise, patient education, and other forms of manual therapy. Non-active interventions included sham techniques (manual contact or detuned modalities) and quiet rest.

Outcome Measure   The primary outcome of interest was a pain sensitivity measure assessing a participant’s response to the application of a quantifiable amount of experimental stimulus. The characteristics of the pain sensitivity measure include the experimental sensory modality used, the psychophysical response, and the location of stimulus application. The experimental sensory modality could include chemical, electrical, ischemic, mechanical (e.g. pressure, vibration), and thermal (e.g. cold, heat) stimuli. Further, these measures could be either static (e.g. threshold or tolerance) or dynamic (e.g. temporal summation) measures of pain processing. Finally, the location of the experimental stimulus application was considered, specifically in relation to the region where SMT was applied. We did not limit inclusion to a specific experimental pain modality as there is no universal stimuli protocol or accepted technique.


      Data Sources

Table 1

Studies were identified by performing a comprehensive systematic literature search for relevant articles in Cumulative Index to Nursing and Allied Health Literature (CINAHL), MEDLINE (PubMed), PsycINFO, and SPORTDiscus from each database’s inception until May 2011. Only manuscripts published in English were included. No limit was placed on the time of publication. Search terms used in the databases included “musculoskeletal manipulations”, “orthopedic manipulation”, “osteopathic manipulation”, “chiropractic manipulation, “manual therapy”, “pain”, “pain measurement”, “pain threshold”, “thermal pain”, “pressure pain”, “mechanical pain”, “experimental pain”, and “exercise-induced pain”. MESH terms (PubMed) and Major Headings (CINAHL) were used when available. Database searches were conducted on May 2, 2011. The search strategy used for the MEDLINE database is listed in Table 1. Additionally, to identify missed studies, we performed a manual search through the reference lists of all potentially relevant articles and previously published systematic reviews.

      Study Search and Selection

The primary author (R.A.C.) screened all articles for eligibility from the search of the databases and reference lists. The initial screening step involved reviewing the article title for potential inclusion into this study. If the title did not provide adequate information for inclusion, abstracts were screened. Articles appearing to meet inclusion criteria based on the screening of title and abstract were considered potentially relevant. Articles deemed not relevant were excluded. After potentially relevant articles were identified, 2 authors (R.A.C. and C.W.G.) independently reviewed the full-texts of these articles for inclusion into the review. Any disagreements regarding article inclusion were resolved by consensus. If consensus could not be reached, a third author (J.E.B.) was recruited to resolve disagreement.

      Data Extraction

Two authors (R.A.C. and C.W.G.) blindly and independently extracted data from each of the included articles with the use of a standardized data extraction form. Results of each author’s extraction were compared to ensure accuracy of the extracted data. Each article was reviewed for the following information: 1.) type of clinical trial; 2.) participant characteristics including age, sex, and clinical condition; 3.) type of intervention within groups including co-intervention and duration of therapy; 4.) pain sensitivity outcome and region in which stimulus was applied; 5.) results of the study (pre- and post- mean values and standard deviation for each measure and each group). The primary author of the respective article was contacted if any of the above information was unobtainable. If the primary author of a study did not provide a response within 7 days of being contacted, the information was not included in the review. Three authors were contacted for information regarding study results and two of three authors provided responses [Bishop et al, 2011; Mansilla-Ferragut et al, 2009].

      Methodological Quality Assessment

The quality of each article was assessed using criteria reported in prior systematic reviews and recommended by the Cochrane Back Review Group [Furlan et al, 2009; Gross et al, 2002; Miller et al, 2010; Rubinstein et al, 2011]. The 12-item criteria allows for assessment of the internal validity of each article (e.g. selection bias, performance bias, attrition bias, detection bias). Articles meeting 6 or more of the 12 items are considered as having low risk of bias (higher quality) [Furlan et al, 2009]. Prior to assessing the quality of the included articles, 2 authors (R.A.C. and C.W.G.) independently scored two trial articles (not included in this analysis) to ensure understanding of the quality criteria. Once understanding was confirmed, the two authors independently rated the quality of each included article. After completion of independent grading, the authors met to finalize the scores for each article. Disagreements regarding article quality were resolved by consensus. If consensus could not be reached, a third author (J.E.B.) was recruited to resolve discrepancy.

      Data Analysis

Microsoft Excel 2007 (Microsoft Corporation, Redmond, WA) and PASW Statistics, Version 18 (SPSS, Inc., Chicago, IL) were used to examine reviewer agreement and individual study descriptive statistics (e.g. mean, standard deviation). For meta-analytical procedures, Comprehensive Meta-analysis, Version 2 (Biostat, Inc., Englewood, NJ) was utilized. Alpha was set at the 0.05 level for statistical significance.

Agreement for the final inclusion of articles (based on examination of full-text) was assessed by kappa statistic and 95% confidence interval for kappa. Agreement for article methodological quality was examined using the intraclass correlation coefficient (ICC) and 95% confidence interval. Both kappa and ICC values range from 0 to 1 with 0 representing no agreement and 1 perfect agreement. We deemed kappa values greater than 0.80 and ICC values greater than 0.75 as excellent [Landis et al, 1977; Portney et al, 2009].

Individual effect size estimates (Hedges g) were generated for each group within each study using information provided in the articles. Each study’s pain sensitivity outcome measure was considered for inclusion into the meta-analysis, however only the immediate effect of SMT on pressure pain threshold (PTT) had an adequate number of studies (e.g. > 2) using similar methodology for further analysis. A random effects model was generated with the primary comparison being the difference in effect on PPT between the group receiving SMT and the comparison group. Hedges g effect size estimates and 95% confidence intervals (CI) were computed as the measure of effect. Effect size estimates were considered small (0.20), medium (0.50), or large (0.80) [Cohen, 1988].

Homogeneity of the estimated effects was tested using a measure of inconsistency (I2) where large values of I2 suggest heterogeneity. Several methods were used to address publication bias [Rothstein et al, 2005]. Publication bias was first examined by observation of a funnel plot. The presence of bias would be indicated in the funnel plot by asymmetry in the effects of individual studies around the overall mean. Egger’s regression method was used to quantify the bias observed. Alpha values < 0.05 indicate significant publication bias. Rosenthal’s failsafe N, the number of missing studies needing to be added to the analysis before the combined effect is non-significant, was computed to indicate whether the observed estimated effect was an artifact of bias. An adjusted overall effect size and 95% CI was computed using Duvall and Tweedie’s trim and fill method.

Additionally, 2 subgroup analyses were performed. We stratified the results by population and location of outcome assessment. The population was categorized as ‘healthy’ (asymptomatic participants) or ‘clinical’ (symptomatic participants) based on the study description. We defined location of outcome assessment as ‘local’ if the pain sensitivity measure was obtained in the same anatomical region to where SMT was applied or ‘remote’ if the pain sensitivity measure was obtained in different anatomical regions from where SMT was applied. In some studies, multiple PPT outcomes were reported. In these cases, PPT measures were combined according to location so as to generate a single composite effect for either local or remote PPT using the methods for combining multiple outcomes described by Borenstein et al [Borenstein, 2009].



RESULTS

      Study Selection

Figure 1

Table 2

Figure 1 depicts a flow diagram of the study selection process with reasons for exclusion at each stage. A total of 1125 articles were identified from the systematic search of CINAHL, MEDLINE, PsycINFO, and SPORTDiscus and 3 articles from a review of reference lists. Once duplicates were removed, 997 articles remained to be assessed for inclusion. Of these, 958 articles were excluded after screening of either the title or abstract. The full-texts of 39 articles were selected to be screened by two independent reviewers. Nineteen articles were excluded based on 1.) study design [Bialosky et al, 2010; Suter et al, 2002], 2.) inability to compare the effects of SMT [Bialosky et al, 2008], 3.) inclusion of non-spinal manipulation or non-thrust manipulation [Brantingham et al, 2005; Fernandez-de-Las Penas et al, 2011; Gamber et al, 2002; Govender et al, 2007; La Touche et al, 2009; Tucker et al, 2003; Vernon et al, 2005; Vicenzino et al, 1998; Vicenzino et al, 1996; Vicenzino et al, 2001; von Piekartz et al, 2011], and 5.) lack of experimental pain outcome [Glover et al, 1974; Godfrey et al, 1984; Hoehler et al, 1981; Keller et al, 2000; Sloop et al, 1982]. As a result, 20 articles representing 20 studies were identified as meeting the criteria for inclusion into this review. The agreement for the included studies was excellent (Kappa = 0.92 [95% CI = 0.83; 1.00]).

      Characteristics of Studies

Table 2 provides a full description of the key characteristics of each study including the characteristics of the sample population, SMT, and pain sensitivity outcome studied.

Sample Population   A total of 974 participants (58% female) were enrolled in the included studies. Eleven studies (n = 695, 59% female) included asymptomatic participants and 9 studies (n = 279, 54% female) included symptomatic participants. The clinical conditions examined within the 9 studies with symptomatic participants were lateral epicondylalgia (1 study) [Fernandez-Carnero et al, 2008], low back pain (2 studies) [Bialosky et al, 2009b; Cote et al, 1994], neck pain (5 studies) [Maduro de Camargo et al, 2011; Mansilla-Ferragut et al, 2009; Parkin-Smith et al, 1998; van Schalkwyk et al, 2000; Vernon et al, 1990], and sacroiliac pain (1 study) [Shearar et al, 2005].The mean age range in studies with asymptomatic participants was 21 to 31 years while the mean age range in studies with symptomatic participants was 30 to 42 years.

Spinal Manipulation   The region targeted for SMT included the cervical spine (11 studies) [Fernandez-Carnero et al, 2008; Fernandez-de-Las-Penas et al, 2008; Fernandez-de-las-Penas et al, 2007; Hamilton et al, 2007; Maduro de Camargo et al, 2011; Mansilla-Ferragut et al, 2009; Oliveira-Campelo et al, 2010; Parkin-Smith et al, 1998; Rubinstein et al, 2011; van Schalkwyk et al, 2000; Vernon et al, 1990], thoracic spine (5 studies) [Bishop et al, 2011; Fryer et al, 2004; Mohammadian et al, 2004; Parkin-Smith et al, 1998; Terrett et al, 1984], and lumbosacral spine (5 studies) [Bialosky et al, 2009b; Cote et al, 1994; George et al, 2006; Shearar et al, 2005; Thomson et al, 2009]. One study examined both cervical and thoracic manipulation [Parkin-Smith et al, 1998]. There was variation betwe/en studies in the manipulation technique(s) used. Of the 11 studies on cervical manipulation, 6 studies [Fernandez-Carnero et al, 2008; Fernandez-de-las-Penas et al, 2007; Maduro de Camargo et al, 2011; Ruiz-Saez et al, 2007; van Schalkwyk et al, 2000; Vernon et al, 1990] examined a middle to lower cervical rotational manipulation while 2 studies [Mansilla-Ferragut et al, 2009; Oliveira-Campelo et al, 2010] examined an upper cervical distraction manipulation. Of the 5 studies on lumbosacral manipulation, 3 studies [Cote et al, 1994; Shearar et al, 2005; Thomson et al, 2009] examined a sidelying rotational manipulation while the other 2 [Bialosky et al, 2009b; George et al, 2006] examined a supine lumbosacral manipulation. It appeared that no 2 studies incorporated the same thoracic manipulation technique.

Pain Sensitivity Outcome   There were different characteristics of the pain sensitivity outcome reported in studies. In terms of sensory modality used, studies investigated responses to chemical [Mohammadian et al, 2004], electrical [Terrett et al, 1984], mechanical [Bishop et al, 2011; Cote et al, 1994; Fernandez-Carnero et al, 2008; Fernandez-de-Las-Penas et al, 2008; Fernandez-de-las-Penas et al, 2007; Fryer et al, 2004; Hamilton et al, 2007; Maduro de Camargo et al, 2011; Mansilla-Ferragut et al, 2009; Oliveira-Campelo et al, 2010; Parkin-Smith et al, 1998; Ruiz-Saez et al, 2007; Shearar et al, 2005; Thomson et al, 2009; van Schalkwyk et al, 2000; Vernon et al, 1990], and thermal stimuli [Bialosky et al, 2009b; Bishop et al, 2011; Fernandez-Carnero et al, 2008; George et al, 2006]. The psychophysical responses examined were primarily a static measure of pain processing, such as a threshold response, while 3 studies [Bialosky et al, 2009b; Bishop et al, 2011; George et al, 2006] examined a dynamic measure, specifically temporal summation of pain.

In the studies that included a mechanical measure, there was similarity in that all these studies examined PPT. However, there was considerable variability in the region in which the pressure stimuli was applied. For example, some of the regions included the cervical spine [Fernandez-de-Las-Penas et al, 2008; Hamilton et al, 2007; Maduro de Camargo et al, 2011; Parkin-Smith et al, 1998; van Schalkwyk et al, 2000; Vernon et al, 1990], elbow [Fernandez-Carnero et al, 2008; Fernandez-de-las-Penas et al, 2007], head region [Mansilla-Ferragut et al, 2009; Oliveira-Campelo et al, 2010], lumbar spine [Bishop et al, 2011; Cote et al, 1994; Shearar et al, 2005; Thomson et al, 2009], trapezius muscle [Maduro de Camargo et al, 2011], and web space of the fingers/toes [Bishop et al, 2011]. All studies, except 3 [Parkin-Smith et al, 1998; Shearar et al, 2005; van Schalkwyk et al, 2000], examined an immediate effect (only 1 session) of SMT.


      Methodological Quality

Table 3

Table 3 summarizes the results for methodological quality of each study. Quality score agreement between the 2 primary raters (R.A.C. and C.W.G.) was excellent with an ICC = 0.79 [95% CI = 0.56; 0.90] and involvement of a third rater was not needed for disagreements. The median score for study quality was 7 with a range from 3 to 8. Three criteria were not met by any study: lack of blinding of the patient (Item 3); lack of blinding of care provider (Item 4); lack of blinding of assessor (Item 5). Information regarding selective outcome reporting (Item 8) for each study was unable to be obtained. Thus, we operationally chose to mark this item for each study as “unsure”.

      Meta-Analysis Results

Figure 2

Figure 3

Of the 20 studies included in this review, only 10 met the criterion for meta-analysis. All 10 studies [Bishop et al, 2011; Cote et al, 1994; Fernandez-de-Las-Penas et al, 2008; Fryer et al, 2004; Hamilton et al, 2007; Maduro de Camargo et al, 2011; Mansilla-Ferragut et al, 2009; Oliveira-Campelo et al, 2010; Ruiz-Saez et al, 2007; Vernon et al, 1990] examined an immediate effect of SMT on PPT. The summary effect estimate suggested a small, but favorable effect of SMT on increasing PPT as compared to other interventions (Hedges g = 0.315 [95% CI = 0.078; 0.552], p = 0.009) (Figure 2). However, heterogeneity was evidenced in the overall model (I2 = 46.9%, p = 0.049).

Six studies [Bishop et al, 2011; Fernandez-de-Las-Penas et al, 2008; Fryer et al, 2004; Hamilton et al, 2007; Oliveira-Campelo et al, 2010; Ruiz-Saez et al, 2007] included asymptomatic participants (healthy population) while 4 studies [Cote et al, 1994; Maduro de Camargo et al, 2011; Mansilla-Ferragut et al, 2009; Vernon et al, 1990] included symptomatic participants (clinical population). The summary effect estimate demonstrated a small favorable, but non-significant effect of SMT on increasing PPT in both the clinical (Hedges g = 0.329 [95% CI = –0.032; 0.691], p = 0.074) and healthy population (Hedges g = 0.337 [95% CI = –0.005; 0.679], p = 0.053) (Figure 3). Heterogeneity was reduced in the clinical population (I2 = 0.0%, p = 0.906), but not in the healthy population (I2 = 69.271, p = 0.006).

Figure 4

Five studies [Cote et al, 1994; Fernandez-de-Las-Penas et al, 2008; Fryer et al, 2004; Hamilton et al, 2007; Vernon et al, 1990] examined PPT at a local body region only, 4 studies [Bishop et al, 2011; Mansilla-Ferragut et al, 2009; Oliveira-Campelo et al, 2010; Ruiz-Saez et al, 2007] examined PPT at a remote body region only, and 1 study [Maduro de Camargo et al, 2011] examined PPT at both a local and remote body region. The summary effect estimate demonstrated a small favorable, but non-significant effect of SMT on increasing PPT at the local site (Hedges g = 0.387 [95% CI = –0.070; 0.844], p = 0.097) (Figure 4). For the remote site, the summary effect estimate demonstrated a small, but significant effect for SMT on increasing PPT (Hedges g = 0.287 [95% CI = 0.073; 0.500], p = 0.008) (Figure 4). Similar to the previous subgroup analysis, heterogeneity was reduced for the remote site studies (I2 = 0.0%, p = 0.858), but not for the local site studies (I2 = 68.057, p = 0.008).

      Publication Bias

Figure 5

Asymmetry was apparent in the funnel plot (Figure 5), especially for studies with less precision (located lower on Y-axis). However, Egger’s test was non-significant (p = 0.09) with intercept = 2.43 [95% CI = –0.482; 5.348]. The failsafe N using a two-tailed criterion was 28. After adjusting for publication bias, the overall effect estimate still demonstrated a small, but significant effect for SMT on increasing PPT (Hedges g = 0.269 [95% CI = 0.106; 0.433].



DISCUSSION

We conducted a comprehensive systematic review and meta-analysis on the hypoalgesic effects of SMT on measures of pain sensitivity. Our meta-analysis results suggest SMT has a favorable effect on increasing PPT, or reducing pain sensitivity, when compared to other forms of intervention. This effect on PPT was largest when measured at a remote anatomical region. These results have implications on potential neurophysiological mechanisms and for areas of future research.

Prior reviews have considered the potential role of SMT on pain processing [Bialosky et al, 2009a; Pickar, 2002; Vernon, 2000]. Many of the studies included in this review were published after the narrative reviews by Pickar [2002] and Vernon [2000]. Our systematic review expands upon these prior works as we were able to 1.) include recent studies published after the reviews by Pickar [2002] and Vernon [2000], 2.) provide information on the quality of SMT studies, and 3.) quantitatively assess the effect of SMT. Overall, we reached a similar conclusion: SMT has a favorable effect on pain sensitivity.

When examining the effect of SMT based on population, there did not appear to be a different effect when studied in healthy versus clinical samples. This is noteworthy as some musculoskeletal conditions have been associated with altered pain sensitivity [Arendt-Nielsen et al, 2010; Fernandez-Carnero et al, 2009; O’Neill et al, 2007]. This difference in pain state does not seem to affect the response to SMT. Studies involving both healthy and clinical participants are important in establishing the mechanisms of SMT. Only 9 of the 20 studies that have evaluated SMT on pain sensitivity responses were assessed among clinical participants. It is imperative that mechanistic studies include clinical participants to link changes in pain sensitivity to changes in a pertinent clinical outcome.

One of the primary questions posed by both Pickar [2002] and Vernon [2000] was whether SMT elicits a general response on pain sensitivity or whether the response is specific to the area where SMT is applied. For example, changes in pain sensitivity over the cervical facets following a cervical spine SMT would indicate a local and specific effect while changes in pain sensitivity in the lumbar facets following a cervical spine SMT would suggest a general effect. We observed a favorable change for increased PPT when measured at remote anatomical sites and a similar, but non-significant change at local anatomical sites. These findings lend support to a possible general effect of SMT beyond the effect expected at the local region of SMT application.

Studies of changes in pain sensitivity in response to SMT indicate potential mechanisms to account for the clinical effectiveness. The mechanisms of SMT are theorized to result from both spinal cord mediated mechanisms [Boal et al, 2004] and supraspinal mediated mechanisms [Wright, 1995]. A recent model of the mechanisms of manual therapy suggests changes in pain related to SMT result from an interaction of neurophysiological responses related to the peripheral nervous system and the central nervous system at the spinal and supraspinal level [Bialosky et al, 2009a]. Prior studies provide support for such an interaction. For example, we have previously observed diminished pain sensitivity to a behavioral measure of dorsal horn excitability (temporal summation of pain) in response to lumbar SMT indicating a spinal cord mediated mechanism [Bialosky et al, 2009b; George et al, 2006]. Interestingly, these findings were reversed in a subsequent study when healthy participants were instructed to expect more pain following the SMT [Bialosky et al, 2008].Collectively these studies suggest an interaction between a spinal cord mediated mechanism of SMT related hypoalgesia (temporal summation) and a supraspinal mediated mechanism related to expectation. Future mechanistic studies of SMT related changes in pain sensitivity should consider and control for potential peripheral, spinal, and supraspinal mechanisms and their potential interaction.

Caution is recommended when interpreting the potential clinical relevance of these findings. While a majority of these studies demonstrated low risk of bias (high quality), 17 of the 20 studies investigated short-term effects only. Collectively, these studies provide evidence that SMT has an immediate effect on reducing pain sensitivity, most notably at the remote region of stimulus assessment with similar results in clinical and healthy populations. Although this an important first step, Cook [Cook, 2011] has highlighted the need for examining the effect of SMT beyond an immediate follow-up since 1.) many interventions (including those considered ineffective) demonstrate a favorable immediate effect, and 2.) it is undetermined how an immediate hypoalgesic effect relates to long-term clinical improvement (e.g. function). Additionally, many of these studies do not link the change in pain sensitivity to a meaningful change in clinical outcome limiting the potential for clinical relevance.

There was a lack of consistency in the pain sensitivity outcomes studied and this allowed for the assessment of effects on PPT only within the meta-analysis. The 20 RCTs included in this review examined the effects of SMT on responses to chemical (n = 1), electrical (n = 1), mechanical (n = 15), and thermal (n = 4) stimuli applied to the skin. However, we observed that only 1 pain sensitivity measure was consistently included in a majority of studies, pressure pain threshold (PPT). Further, the experimental stimuli tended to be applied and measured at 1 anatomical region only. Future studies investigating the mechanisms of hypoalgesia related to SMT should consider assessing pain sensitivity with multiple experimental sensory stimuli at local and remote sites. These assessments are useful for characterizing whether SMT, and other forms of manual therapy, have 1.) a robust hypoalgesic effect (e.g. across multiples sensory modalities), 2.) effects that are specific to the location of application and/or the location of pain (for clinical samples), and 3.) effects that include a central nervous system component of modulating afferent nociceptive signal (e.g. remote effects).

Our review has additional implications for future studies. First, while it was beyond the scope of this study to examine dosage, we noted that either too little information was provided by authors regarding dosage or no attempt was made to examine the effect of dosage on pain sensitivity responses. Understanding the effect of repeated SMT application is relevant as no optimal dosage has been appreciated in mechanistic or clinical studies. Second, there was no consensus among the studies on type of stimulus or parameters of stimulus application. Few studies incorporated more than 1 stimulus modality or multi-regional application of the stimulus. Using multiple stimuli for studying pain in both a clinical and research setting has been recommended by previous authors [Arendt-Nielsen et al, 2009; Hastie et al, 2005; Nijs et al, 2009]. Pain sensitivity may differ based on type of stimulus and there is potential for clinical and healthy participants to exhibit different pain profiles [Hastie et al, 2005]. By implementing this, future research could consider whether SMT results in a change in global pain sensitivity or modality-specific sensitivity.

Limitations

There are several limitations in this review. While most of the studies demonstrated low risk of bias, there is the potential for some of the scoring items to be irrelevant as 17 studies were immediate effect studies. For example, within an immediate effect study, there is little concern for drop-outs (Item 6), compliance (Item 11), and timing of assessment (Item 12). Thus, the qualitative scoring criteria may be overestimated for these studies. Another limitation includes the lack of unpublished studies entered into this review. It is undetermined how the inclusion of these reports would impact the overall results. Further, we did not obtain information regarding concomitant clinical pain reports following SMT. Our focus remained on pain sensitivity responses rather than clinical pain. Prior reviews have examined the effect of SMT and other forms of manual therapy on clinical pain complaints and thus this paper is best viewed as a mechanistic investigation. In this review, we chose to combine studies in the meta-analysis that exhibited some heterogeneity. For example, we aimed to examine the response of SMT globally on any site of PPT measure and we did not have enough studies to examine each region of SMT separately. Despite this, we did stratify the analysis by location of PPT.



CONCLUSION

The mechanism of SMT remains elusive, but SMT appears to modulate pain through both central and peripheral pathways. Studies have investigated the effect of SMT using variable experimental pain modalities including chemical, electrical, mechanical, and thermal stimuli. SMT demonstrated a favorable effect over other interventions on pressure pain thresholds (PPT). Additionally, subgroup analysis showed a significant effect of SMT on remote sites of pressure stimulus application further supporting a potential influence on higher levels within the central nervous system. Future studies using experimental pain testing to examine the mechanisms of SMT should include multiple stimuli and test at multiple anatomical sites if determining potential mechanisms is the goal.


Acknowledgments

We would like to acknowledge Jason M. Beneciuk PT, DPT, FAAOMPT and Corey B. Simon PT, DPT, FAAOMPT for their review of this manuscript. This manuscript was written while CWG received funding from the University of Florida Alumni Fellowship and NCMIC Foundation Educational Grant. JEB received support from the Rehabilitation Research Career Development Program (5K12HD055929-02). MDB and SZG received support from the National Center for Complementary and Alternative Medicine (R01AT006334).


References:

  1. Arendt-Nielsen L, Nie H, Laursen MB, et al.
    Sensitization in patients with painful knee osteoarthritis.
    Pain. 2010;149(3):573–581

  2. Arendt-Nielsen L, Yarnitsky D.
    Experimental and clinical applications of quantitative sensory testing applied to skin, muscles and viscera.
    Journal of Pain. 2009;10(6):556–572

  3. Bialosky JE, Bishop MD, Price DD, Robinson ME, George SZ.
    The Mechanisms of Manual Therapy in the Treatment of Musculoskeletal Pain:
    A Comprehensive Model

    Man Ther. 2009 (Oct); 14 (5): 531–538

  4. Bialosky JE, Bishop MD, Robinson ME, Barabas JA, George SZ.
    The Influence of Expectation on Spinal Manipulation Induced Hypoalgesia:
    An Experimental Study in Normal Subjects

    BMC Musculoskelet Disord. 2008 (Feb 11); 9: 19

  5. Bialosky JE, Bishop MD, Robinson ME, George SZ.
    The relationship of the audible pop to hypoalgesia associated with high-velocity, low-amplitude thrust manipulation: a secondary analysis of an experimental study in pain-free participants.
    Journal of Manipulative & Physiological Therapeutics. 2010;33(2):117–124

  6. Bialosky, J.E., Bishop, M.D., Robinson, M.E., Zeppieri, G. Jr, and George, S.Z.
    Spinal Manipulative Therapy Has an Immediate Effect on Thermal Pain Sensitivity
    in People With Low Back Pain: A Randomized Controlled Trial

    Phys Ther. 2009 (Dec); 89 (12): 1292–1303

  7. Bishop MD, Beneciuk JM, George SZ.
    Immediate reduction in temporal sensory summation after thoracic spinal manipulation.
    The Spine Journal. 2011

  8. Blumenstiel K, Gerhardt A, Rolke R, et al.
    Quantitative Sensory Testing Profiles in Chronic Back Pain Are Distinct From Those in Fibromyalgia.
    Clinical Journal of Pain. 2011

  9. Boal RW, Gillette RG.
    Central neuronal plasticity, low back pain and spinal manipulative therapy.
    Journal of Manipulative & Physiological Therapeutics. 2004;27(5):314–326

  10. Borenstein M.
    Introduction to meta-analysis.
    Chichester, U.K: John Wiley & Sons; 2009

  11. Brantingham JW, Guiry S, Kretzmann HH, Kite VJ, Globe G.
    A pilot study of the efficacy of a conservative chiropractic protocol using graded mobilization, manipulation and ice in the treatment of symptomatic hallux abductovalgus bunion.
    Clinical Chiropractic. 2005;8(3):117–133

  12. Childs JD, Fritz JM, Flynn TW, Irrgang JJ, Johnson KK, Majkowski GR, et al.
    A Clinical Prediction Rule To Identify Patients With Low Back Pain Most Likely To Benefit
    from Spinal Manipulation: A Validation Study

    Annals of Internal Medicine 2004 (Dec 21); 141 (12): 920–928

  13. Chua NH, van Suijlekom HA, Vissers KC, Arendt-Nielsen L, Wilder-Smith OH.
    Differences in sensory processing between chronic cervical zygapophysial joint pain patients with and without cervicogenic headache.
    Cephalalgia. 2011;31(8):953–963

  14. Cohen J.
    Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. 2.
    Hillsdale, N.J: L. Erlbaum Associates; 1988

  15. Cook CE.
    Immediate effects from manual therapy: much ado about nothing?
    Journal of Manual & Manipulative Therapy. 2011;19(1):3–4

  16. Cote P, Mior SA, Vernon H.
    The short-term effect of a spinal manipulation on pain/pressure threshold in patients with chronic mechanical low back pain.
    Journal of Manipulative & Physiological Therapeutics. 1994;17(6):364–368

  17. Evans DW.
    Mechanisms and Effects of Spinal High-velocity, Low-amplitude Thrust Manipulation: Previous Theories
    Manipulative Physiol Ther. 2002 (May); 25 (4): 251–262

  18. Fernandez-Carnero J, Fernandez-de-las-Penas C, Cleland JA.
    Immediate hypoalgesic and motor effects after a single cervical spine manipulation in subjects with lateral epicondylalgia.
    Journal of Manipulative & Physiological Therapeutics. 2008;31(9):675–681

  19. Fernandez-Carnero J, Fernandez-de-Las-Penas C, de la Llave-Rincon AI, Ge HY, Arendt-Nielsen L.
    Widespread mechanical pain hypersensitivity as sign of central sensitization in unilateral epicondylalgia: a blinded, controlled study.
    Clin J Pain. 2009;25(7):555–561

  20. Fernandez-de-Las-Penas C, Alonso-Blanco C, Cleland JA, Rodriguez-Blanco C, Alburquerque-Sendin F.
    Changes in pressure pain thresholds over C5-C6 zygapophyseal joint after a cervicothoracic junction manipulation in healthy subjects.
    Journal of Manipulative & Physiological Therapeutics. 2008;31(5):332–337

  21. Fernández-De-Las-Peñas C, Pérez-De-Heredia M.
    Immediate Effects on Pressure Pain Threshold Following a Single Cervical Spine Manipulation
    in Healthy Subjects

    J Orthop Sports Phys Ther. 2007 (Jun); 37 (6): 325–329

  22. Fernandez-de-Las Penas C, Cleland JA, Palomeque-del-Cerro L, Caminero A, Guillem-Mesado A, Jimenez-Garcia R.
    Development of a clinical prediction rule for identifying women with tension-type headache who are likely to achieve short-term success with joint mobilization and muscle trigger point therapy.
    Headache: The Journal of Head & Face Pain. 2011;51(2):246–261

  23. Fryer G, Carub J, McIver S.
    The effect of manipulation and mobilisation on pressure pain thresholds in the thoracic spine.
    Journal of Osteopathic Medicine. 2004;7(1):8–14

  24. Furlan AD, Pennick V, Bombardier C, van Tulder M.
    2009 updated method guidelines for systematic reviews in the Cochrane Back Review Group.
    Spine (Phila Pa 1976) 2009;34(18):1929–1941

  25. Gamber RG, Shores JH, Russo DP, Jimenez C, Rubin BR.
    Osteopathic manipulative treatment in conjunction with medication relieves pain associated with fibromyalgia syndrome: results of a randomized clinical pilot project.
    Journal of the American Osteopathic Association. 2002;102(6):321–325

  26. George SZ, Bishop MD, Bialosky JE, Zeppieri G, Jr, Robinson ME.
    Immediate effects of spinal manipulation on thermal pain sensitivity: an experimental study.
    BMC Musculoskeletal Disorders. 2006;7:68

  27. Giesecke T, Gracely RH, Grant MA, et al.
    Evidence of augmented central pain processing in idiopathic chronic low back pain.
    Arthritis & Rheumatism. 2004;50(2):613–623

  28. Glover JR, Morris JG, Khosla T.
    Back pain: a randomized clinical trial of rotational manipulation of the trunk.
    British Journal of Industrial Medicine. 1974;31(1):59–64

  29. Godfrey CM, Morgan PP, Schatzker J.
    A randomized trial of manipulation for low-back pain in a medical setting.
    Spine (Phila Pa 1976) 1984;9(3):301–304

  30. Govender N, Kretzmann H, Price JL, Brantingham JW, Globe G.
    A single-blinded randomized placebo-controlled clinical trial of manipulation and mobilization in the treatment of Morton’s neuroma.
    Journal of the American Chiropractic Association. 2007;44(3):8–18

  31. Gross AR, Kay T, Hondras M, et al.
    Manual therapy for mechanical neck disorders: a systematic review.
    Manual Therapy. 2002;7(3):131–149

  32. Hamilton L, Boswell C, Fryer G.
    The effects of high-velocity, low-amplitude manipulation and muscle energy technique on suboccipital tenderness.
    International Journal of Osteopathic Medicine. 2007;10(2–3):42–49

  33. Hastie BA, Riley JL, 3rd, Robinson ME, et al.
    Cluster analysis of multiple experimental pain modalities.
    Pain. 2005;116(3):227–237

  34. Hoehler FK, Tobis JS, Buerger AA.
    Spinal manipulation for low back pain.
    Journal of the American Medical Association. 1981;245(18):1835–1838

  35. Iverson CA, Sutlive TG, Crowell MS, et al.
    Lumbopelvic Manipulation for the Treatment of Patients With Patellofemoral Pain Syndrome: Development of a Clinical Prediction Rule.
    Journal of Orthopaedic & Sports Physical Therapy. 2008;38(6):297–312

  36. Keller TS, Colloca CJ:
    Mechanical Force Spinal Manipulation Increases Trunk Muscle Strength
    Assessed By Electromyography: A Comparative Clinical Trial

    J Manipulative Physiol Ther. 2000 (Nov);   23 (9):   585–595

  37. La Touche R, Fernandez-de-las-Penas C, Fernandez-Carnero J, et al.
    The effects of manual therapy and exercise directed at the cervical spine on pain and pressure pain sensitivity in patients with myofascial temporomandibular disorders.
    Journal of Oral Rehabilitation. 2009;36(9):644–652

  38. Landis JR, Koch GG.
    The measurement of observer agreement for categorical data.
    Biometrics. 1977;33(1):159–174

  39. Liberati A, Altman DG, Tetzlaff J, et al.
    The PRISMA statement for reporting systematic reviews and meta-analyses of studies that evaluate health care interventions: explanation and elaboration.
    Annals of Internal Medicine. 2009;151(4):W65–94

  40. Maduro de Camargo V, Alburquerque-Sendin F, Berzin F, Stefanelli VC, De Souza DP, Fernandez-de-Las Penas C.
    Immediate effects on electromyographic activity and pressure pain thresholds after a cervical manipulation in mechanical neck pain: a randomized controlled trial.
    Journal of Manipulative & Physiological Therapeutics. 2011

  41. Mansilla-Ferragut P, Fernandez-de-Las Penas C, Alburquerque-Sendin F, Cleland JA, Bosca-Gandia JJ.
    Immediate effects of atlanto-occipital joint manipulation on active mouth opening and pressure pain sensitivity in women with mechanical neck pain.
    Journal of Manipulative & Physiological Therapeutics. 2009;32(2):101–106

  42. Miller J, Gross A, D'Sylva J, et al.
    Manual Therapy and Exercise for Neck Pain: A Systematic Review
    Man Ther. 2010 (Aug); 15 (4): 334–354

  43. Mintken PE, Cleland JA, Carpenter KJ, Bieniek ML, Keirns M, Whitman JM.
    Some factors predict successful short-term outcomes in individuals with shoulder pain receiving cervicothoracic manipulation: a single-arm trial.
    Physical Therapy. 2010;90(1):26–42

  44. Mohammadian P, Gonsalves A, Tsai C, Hummel T, Carpenter T.
    Areas of Capsaicin-Induced Secondary Hyperalgesia and Allodynia Are Reduced by a
    Single Chiropractic Adjustment: A Preliminary Study

    J Manipulative Physiol Ther. 2004 (Jul);   27 (6): 381–387

  45. Nahin RL. Barnes PM. Stussman BJ. Bloom B.
    Costs of Complementary and Alternative Medicine (CAM)
    and Frequency of Visits to CAM Practitioners:
    United States, 2007

    National Health Statistics Reports 2009 (Jul 30); (18): 1–14

  46. Nijs J, Van Houdenhove B, Oostendorp RA.
    Recognition of central sensitization in patients with musculoskeletal pain: Application of pain neurophysiology in manual therapy practice.
    Manual Therapy. 2009

  47. O’Neill S, Manniche C, Graven-Nielsen T, Arendt-Nielsen L.
    Generalized deep-tissue hyperalgesia in patients with chronic low-back pain.
    European Journal of Pain. 2007;11(4):415–420

  48. Oliveira-Campelo NM, Rubens-Rebelatto J, Marti NVFJ, Alburquerque-Sendi NF, Fernandez-de-Las-Penas C.
    The immediate effects of atlanto-occipital joint manipulation and suboccipital muscle inhibition technique on active mouth opening and pressure pain sensitivity over latent myofascial trigger points in the masticatory muscles.
    Journal of Orthopaedic & Sports Physical Therapy. 2010;40(5):310–317

  49. Parkin-Smith GF, Penter CS.
    A clinical trial investigating the effect of two manipulative approaches in the treatment of mechanical neck pain: a pilot study.
    Journal of the Neuromusculoskeletal System. 1998;6(1):6–16

  50. Pickar JG.
    Neurophysiological Effects of Spinal Manipulation
    Spine J (N American Spine Society) 2002 (Sep); 2 (5): 357–371

  51. Portney LG, Watkins MP.
    Foundations of clinical research : applications to practice. 3.
    Upper Saddle River, N.J: Pearson/Prentice Hall; 2009

  52. Rothstein H, Sutton AJ, Borenstein M.
    Publication bias in meta-analysis : prevention, assessment and adjustments.
    Chichester, England ; Hoboken, NJ: Wiley; 2005

  53. Rubinstein SM, van Middelkoop M, Assendelft WJ, de Boer MR, van Tulder MW.
    Spinal Manipulative Therapy for Chronic Low-Back Pain: An Update of a Cochrane Review.
    Spine (Phila Pa 1976) 2011;36(13):E825–E846

  54. Ruiz-Saez M, Fernandez-de-las-Penas C, Blanco CR,
    Martinez-Segura R, Garcia-Leon R.
    Changes in Pressure Pain Sensitivity in Latent Myofascial Trigger Points in the
    Upper Trapezius Muscle After a Cervical Spine Manipulation in Pain-Free Subjects

    J Manipulative Physiol Ther. 2007 (Oct); 30 (8): 578–583

  55. Shearar KA, Colloca CJ, White HL.
    A randomized clinical trial of manual versus mechanical force manipulation in the treatment of sacroiliac joint syndrome.
    Journal of Manipulative & Physiological Therapeutics. 2005;28(7):493–501

  56. Sloop PR, Smith DS, Goldenberg E, Dore C.
    Manipulation for chronic neck pain. A double-blind controlled study.
    Spine (Phila Pa 1976) 1982;7(6):532–535

  57. Staahl C, Drewes AM.
    Experimental human pain models: a review of standardised methods for preclinical testing of analgesics.
    Basic & Clinical Pharmacology & Toxicology. 2004;95(3):97–111

  58. Staahl C, Olesen AE, Andresen T, Arendt-Nielsen L, Drewes AM.
    Assessing analgesic actions of opioids by experimental pain models in healthy volunteers - an updated review.
    British Journal of Clinical Pharmacology. 2009a;68(2):149–168

  59. Staahl C, Olesen AE, Andresen T, Arendt-Nielsen L, Drewes AM.
    Assessing efficacy of non-opioid analgesics in experimental pain models in healthy volunteers: an updated review.
    British Journal of Clinical Pharmacology. 2009b;68(3):322–341

  60. Staud R.
    Is it all central sensitization? Role of peripheral tissue nociception in chronic musculoskeletal pain.
    Current Rheumatology Reports. 2010;12(6):448–454

  61. Suter E, McMorland G.
    Decrease in elbow flexor inhibition after cervical spine manipulation in patients with chronic neck pain.
    Clinical Biomechanics. 2002;17(7):541–544

  62. Terrett ACJ, Vernon H.
    Manipulation and pain tolerance: a controlled study of the effect of spinal manipulation on paraspinal cutaneous pain tolerance levels.
    American Journal of Physical Medicine. 1984;63(5):217–225

  63. Thomson O, Haig L, Mansfield H.
    The effects of high-velocity low-amplitude thrust manipulation and mobilisation techniques on pressure pain threshold in the lumbar spine.
    International Journal of Osteopathic Medicine. 2009;12(2):56–62

  64. Tucker M, Brantingham JW, Myburg C.
    Relative effectiveness of a non-steroidal anti-inflammatory medication (Meloxicam) versus manipulation in the treatment of osteo-arthritis of the knee.
    European Journal of Chiropractic. 2003;50(3):163–183

  65. Valencia C, Fillingim RB, George SZ.
    Suprathreshold heat pain response is associated with clinical pain intensity for patients with shoulder pain.
    Journal of Pain. 2011;12(1):133–140

  66. van Schalkwyk R, Parkin-Smith GF.
    A clinical trial investigating the possible effect of the supine cervical rotatory manipulation and the supine lateral break manipulation in the treatment of mechanical neck pain: a pilot study.
    Journal of Manipulative & Physiological Therapeutics. 2000;23(5):324–331

  67. Vernon, H.
    Qualitative Review of Studies of Manipulation-induced Hypoalgesia
    J Manipulative Physiol Ther 2000 (Feb); 23 (2): 134–138

  68. Vernon H, MacAdam K, Marshall V, Pion M, Sadowska M.
    Validation of a Sham Manipulative Procedure for the Cervical Spine for Use in Clinical Trials
    J Manipulative Physiol Ther 2005 (Nov); 28 (9): 662–666

  69. Vernon HT, Aker P, Burns S, Viljakaanen S, Short L.
    Pressure pain threshold evaluation of the effect of spinal manipulation in the treatment of chronic neck pain: a pilot study.
    Journal of Manipulative & Physiological Therapeutics. 1990;13(1):13–16

  70. Vicenzino B, Collins D, Benson H, Wright A.
    An investigation of the interrelationship between manipulative therapy-induced hypoalgesia and sympathoexcitation.
    Journal of Manipulative & Physiological Therapeutics. 1998;21(7):448–453

  71. Vicenzino B, Collins D, Wright A.
    The initial effects of a cervical spine manipulative physiotherapy treatment on the pain and dysfunction of lateral epicondylalgia.
    Pain. 1996;68(1):69–74

  72. Vicenzino B, Paungmali A, Buratowski S, Wright A.
    Specific manipulative therapy treatment for chronic lateral epicondylalgia produces uniquely characteristic hypoalgesia.
    Manual Therapy. 2001;6(4):205–212

  73. von Piekartz H, Ludtke K.
    Effect of treatment of temporomandibular disorders (TMD) in patients with cervicogenic headache: a single-blind, randomized controlled study.
    Cranio. 2011;29(1):43–56

  74. Wallin M, Liedberg G, Borsbo B, Gerdle B.
    Thermal Detection and Pain Thresholds but not Pressure Pain Thresholds Are Correlated With Psychological Factors in Women With Chronic Whiplash-Associated Pain.
    Clinical Journal of Pain. 2011

  75. Werner MU, Mjobo HN, Nielsen PR, Rudin A.
    Prediction of postoperative pain: a systematic review of predictive experimental pain studies.
    Anesthesiology. 2010;112(6):1494–1502

  76. Wright A.
    Hypoalgesia post-manipulative therapy: a review of a potential neurophysiological mechanism.
    Manual Therapy. 1995;1(1):11–16

Return to SUBLUXATION

Return to SPINAL PAIN MANAGEMENT

Since 10-03-2015

                  © 1995–2024 ~ The Chiropractic Resource Organization ~ All Rights Reserved