PATIENT EXPECTATIONS AS PREDICTORS OF OUTCOME IN PATIENTS WITH ACUTE LOW BACK PAIN
 
   

Patient Expectations as Predictors of Outcome
in Patients with Acute Low Back Pain

This section was compiled by Frank M. Painter, D.C.
Send all comments or additions to:
   Frankp@chiro.org
 
   

FROM:   J General Internal Medicine 2008 (Feb); 23 (2): 148–153 ~ FULL TEXT

  OPEN ACCESS   


Samuel S. Myers MD, Russell S. Phillips MD, Roger B. Davis ScD, Daniel C. Cherkin PhD, Anna Legedza ScD, Ted J. Kaptchuk, Andrea Hrbek, Julie E. Buring ScD, Diana Post MD, Maureen T. Connelly MD, MPH & David M. Eisenberg MD

Department of Medicine,
Harvard Medical School,
Mount Auburn Hospital,
Cambridge, MA 02138, USA.



BACKGROUND:   Few studies have evaluated the association between patient expectations for recovery and clinical outcomes, and no study has evaluated whether asking patients to choose their therapy modifies such an association.

OBJECTIVE:   To evaluate the association between patients' expectations and functional recovery in patients with acute low back pain (LBP), and to determine whether that association is affected by giving patients choice of therapy.

DESIGN AND PARTICIPANTS:   A secondary analysis of a randomized controlled trial comparing usual care alone to usual care plus choice of chiropractic, acupuncture, or massage in 444 adults with acute LBP, lasting less than 21 days.

MEASUREMENTS AND MAIN RESULTS:   Primary outcome was functional disability (Roland score) at 5 and 12 weeks. Patients' general expectations for improvement were associated with improvement in functional status (beta = 0.96, 95% CI = 0.56, 1.36). A 1-point increase in general expectations was associated with a 0.96-point improvement in Roland score. The association of expectation with outcome was 2-3 times greater in the usual care group than the choice group. However, these differences did not reach statistical significance.

There are more articles like this @ our:

PATIENT EXPECTATIONS OF RELIEF Page

CONCLUSIONS:   In patients with acute LBP, higher expectations for recovery are associated with greater functional improvement. Eliciting patient expectations for improvement may be a simple way to identify patients with the highest (or lowest) likelihood of experiencing functional improvement. Incorporating questions about patient expectations in future trials may clarify the role of this important correlate of clinical outcomes.

KEY WORDS:   expectations, preference, choice, low back pain, randomized controlled trial



From the Full-Text Article:

BACKGROUND

Prior research has documented intriguing associations between patients’ expectations for recovery and their clinical outcomes. [1–8] A systematic review of the literature on expectations of recovery found that 15 of 16 studies reported that positive patient expectations were associated with better health outcomes. [9]

Most studies evaluating the association between expectations and clinical outcomes have focused on relatively subjective outcomes. Patients with high expectations that acupressure bands would provide relief from chemotherapy-induced nausea reported more relief than those with low expectations, [10] And patients with benign prostatic hypertrophy who had positive expectations for surgery reported feeling better after surgery than patients with more negative expectations. [2]

Regarding functional outcomes, investigators found that general expectations for recovery were associated with functional ability after total joint arthroplasty. [11] Higher expectations of specific therapies, but not general expectations for improvement, were associated with functional improvement in patients with persistent low back pain. [4]

Because of its clinical relevance, and its relevance to future study design, it is important to expand our understanding of this association. Are patients who are generally optimistic about their recovery more likely to get better than patients who are more pessimistic? Are there sociodemographic or clinical factors associated with having high expectations? Are there other factors that modify the effect of expectations on outcome?

In this context, we performed a secondary analysis to evaluate whether there was an association between patients’ general expectations for recovery from acute low back pain (ALBP) and their functional outcomes. We also evaluated whether patients’ expectations of specific therapies were associated with their outcomes. In addition, we took advantage of a unique element of the parent study’s design to examine whether the effect of expectation on outcome was modified by asking patients to choose their therapy. Because one possible explanation for an association between expectation and outcome is that expectation is a component of the placebo effect, we hypothesized that this effect might be different among patients who chose their own therapy versus those who were prescribed therapy by their clinicians. To our knowledge, this question of a differential effect of expectations based on giving patients choice of therapy has never been studied.



METHODS

We performed a secondary analysis of data from a randomized controlled clinical trial comparing usual care alone to usual care plus patient choice of adjunctive complementary and alternative medical (CAM) therapy (consisting of acupuncture, chiropractic, or massage) for acute low back pain (ALBP). We recruited patients from 4 clinical practice sites of Harvard Vanguard Medical Associates (HVMA), a large multispecialty group practice in the greater Boston area. Full details of recruitment have been previously described.v12

      PARTICIPANTS

English-speaking patients age 18 and above were considered for the study if they were presenting for initial evaluation of low back pain and scored greater than 3 on a 0–10 pain scale. We excluded patients whose pain was not in the low back or had lasted longer than 21 days. Other reasons for exclusion have been described. [12]

      RANDOMIZATION

We randomized eligible participants at each clinic using a stratified permuted-block design by clinical site. Randomization assigned one-third of participants to the usual care arm.

      TREATMENTS

Clinics provided usual care to all participants according to their standard treatment algorithm, which included NSAIDs, muscle relaxants, limited bed rest, education, and activity alterations. Participants randomized to choice continued to receive usual care at HVMA clinics, but were also given a choice of adjunctive acupuncture, chiropractic, or massage therapy. CAM treatments took place at the private offices of participating credentialed CAM providers. Further details about treatments provided have been described. [12]

      DATA COLLECTION

At baseline interviews, we obtained data on patient demographics including age, gender, education, income, race, marital status, employment status, and smoking. We collected data on patient expectations as described below. We assessed baseline functional status using the modified Roland–Morris Disability Questionnairev [13–15] that includes 23 yes–no questions about difficulties with daily activities such as difficulty getting dressed and climbing stairs. The overall score is a sum of positive responses, ranging from 0 to 23, with a higher score signifying more disability. The instrument has been extensively validated. A change in score of 2 points or greater is considered clinically significant. [13] We also evaluated clinical status, disability, history of back pain, general health, and depressive symptoms as previously described. [12] Our primary outcome was change in functional status from baseline to 5 and 12 weeks.

We analyzed 2 measures of patient expectation, collected at baseline. The first was the participants’ general expectation based on the question: “Using a scale from 0 to 10, with 0 being no improvement and 10 being complete recovery, how much improvement do you expect in six weeks?” The second expectation variable was a participant’s specific expectation of the CAM therapy that they chose. Before randomization, patients were asked: “Using a scale from 0 to 10 (where 0 is not at all helpful and 10 is extremely helpful), how helpful do you believe that _______ [a specified CAM therapy] would be for your current episode of back pain or sciatica?” For each participant, the number they assigned to the therapy that they ultimately chose was used as their measure of specific expectation. General expectation was measured for all participants, whereas specific expectation was limited to those randomized to the choice group.

      DATA ANALYSIS

In the main study comparing usual care (UC) with choice of CAM therapy for ALBP, the choice group showed no difference in functional status at week 5.12 For our initial analysis, we pooled data from the UC and choice groups to maximize power.

We used linear regression to determine whether expectations were independently associated with a 5-week or 12-week change in Roland score. We first tested the continuous expectation variables for an association with change in Roland score using unadjusted linear regression. We also tested each of the sociodemographic, clinical, and historical covariates for an association with the primary outcome using unadjusted analysis. Because our primary focus was the unadjusted analysis, we only ran adjusted models if a relationship was seen in the unadjusted analysis.

In the adjusted analysis, we included those covariates found to have an association with the outcome with a p value of ?.10 in the unadjusted analysis. We attempted to build a parsimonious model by using a forward selection algorithm followed by backward elimination to remove variables no longer significant at p < .05. We individually tested those covariates excluded from the model for confounding, and would have reincluded any covariates whose inclusion produced a change of 10% or more in the estimated coefficient for the expectation variable of interest. However, none of the covariates produced such an effect in either model. We performed all analyses with SAS 9.1.3 (2002–2003 by SAS Institute Inc., Cary, NC, USA).

If we found an association between either expectation measure and outcome, we stratified that expectation variable into 4 categories: low (0–2), medium (3–6), high (7–9), and very high (10). We evaluated the distribution of sociodemographic and clinical covariates by expectation category to see if there were any significant differences in distribution. For ordinal variables, we tested for significance using a Kruskal–Wallis test. For binary variables, we used a Cochrane–Armitage test for trend. For continuous variables, we tested for a trend across the different means using a linear contrast within linear regression.

In a secondary analysis, we stratified participants according to whether they were in the usual care or choice groups to see if there was any difference in the association between expectation and outcome in these two groups. We used the same adjusted linear regression models we constructed for the general study population and analyzed the association between general expectation and primary outcome in the usual care and choice groups separately. To test for effect modification we introduced an interaction term (the product of the usual care vs choice variable with the expectation variable) in our models.



RESULTS

Table 1

As shown in Table 1, our population tended to be white, well-educated, and employed. Thirty-four percent of our patients were seeing a physician for ALBP for the first time. Mean pain and Roland scores represented moderate to high levels of pain and dysfunction. Over a 5-week period, mean Roland score dropped more than 50%. Attitudes toward CAM therapies appeared to be similar to attitudes toward conventional physical therapy. On the 0–10 scale, patients, on average, rated the probable helpfulness of chiropractic at 6.1 (±2.96), acupuncture 6.1 (±2.52), and massage 7.2 (±2.27). They rated physical therapy 6.9 (±2.51).

Table 2

In the unadjusted analysis, general expectation, but not specific expectation, was strongly and significantly associated with improvement in Roland scores at 5 and 12 weeks. For each 1-point increase in general expectation, there was a 0.96-point improvement in Roland score at 5 weeks. In the adjusted models, general expectation retained statistical significance (see Table 2).

As seen in Table 1, patients’ general expectations tended to be high. When we stratified patients according to level of general expectation, no participants fell into the low category. Thirteen percent of participants (58) reported their general expectations for improvement in the medium range, 42% (186) reported expectations in the high range, and 45% (198) reported expectations for improvement as 10 on a 0–10 scale. The mean general expectation was 8.6 (standard deviation [SD] = 1.7).

There were no significant differences in the distribution of age, sex, income, education, race, or employment status by general expectation category. However, married patients or those with a partner had higher expectations. Other factors associated with higher general expectations included: worse pain or function at baseline; better general health; seeing a health care provider for ALBP for the first time; or finding one’s primary care physician or physical therapist to have been helpful for a prior episode of ALBP.
Table 3

Table 4

In the adjusted models, in addition to general expectation, age, race, income, depressive symptoms, baseline Roland score, sciatica, and first time seeing an MD for back pain were independent correlates with functional status at both 5 and 12 weeks (see Table 3).

In a secondary, stratified analysis, we found that the magnitude of the association between general expectation and change in Roland score at 5 weeks (?-coefficient) was nearly twice as large in the usual care group (0.95) as the choice group (0.55) (Table 4). At 12 weeks, this difference was even more pronounced between usual care (0.80) and choice (0.29). When an interaction term (treatment group by expectation) was entered into the model, however, it did not reach significance for a 5-week Roland score (p?=?0.27) and was just short of significance for a 12-week Roland score (p?=?0.068).



DISCUSSION

We found that participants’ general expectations for improvement, but not their specific expectations of chosen therapies, were significantly associated with changes in disability at 5 and 12 weeks. We also found that the association between general expectations and outcome appeared to be substantially higher in the usual care versus the choice group (see Table 4).

We are aware of only 1 prior study that has evaluated the effect of patient expectations on their low back pain outcomes. In that study, general expectations for improvement were not found to be associated with improvement in Roland score at 10 weeks, whereas specific expectations were associated with improvement.4 The studies, however, had important differences. Whereas our population suffered from acute back pain, the prior study evaluated patients with chronic back pain. The prior study was limited to 135 patients and used a 7-point Likert scale (where patients rated their expectation for recovery in 1 month from 1 [complete recovery] to 7 [much worse]) to measure general expectation. A final, fundamental difference was that the prior study randomly assigned patients to acupuncture or chiropractic, whereas our study asked patients to choose between three CAM therapies. In this light, the possible mediating effect of choice on the association between expectation and outcome (discussed below) may be relevant.

The different measures of general expectation in these 2 studies highlight the need for a more standardized approach to studying the association between patient expectations and clinical outcomes. In the systematic review performed by Mondloch and colleagues, only 2 of the 16 studies reviewed shared a common question regarding patient expectations. [9] To develop a more comprehensive understanding of the complex associations between patient expectations and recovery, we need to develop validated instruments for assessing expectations and take a more uniform approach in our study designs, asking study subjects about expectations before randomization.

Whereas the differences between studies on patient expectations are noteworthy, the similarities are even more striking. In the vast majority of studies, investigators have consistently shown strong, statistically and clinically significant associations between patients’ expectations and clinical recovery.

The reason for this association remains unexplained. It could be that patients are good judges of their own illnesses and are able to accurately identify their likelihood of improving over several weeks. An alternative explanation is that there is a reporting bias such that patients who have predicted they will have significant improvement are more likely to report improvement several weeks later so as to be self-consistent. A third possibility is that higher expectations are associated with better compliance with a medical regimen that is responsible for greater improvement. It is also possible that there is something about the expectation itself that is therapeutic, akin to the placebo effect.

One of the more intriguing, albeit speculative, findings of our study is the possible differential effect of expectation on outcome in the choice versus the usual care groups (see Table 4). To our knowledge, no study has ever looked at this type of effect modification. Whereas it is possible that such a difference could result from patients having strong negative associations with CAM (and thereby having their expectations dampened after being randomized to choice), this does not appear to have been the case. On average, we found that patients rated CAM therapies similar to conventional physical therapy in terms of their likely helpfulness for their current episode of back pain.

The stratified analysis could shed light on the possible etiologic scenarios outlined above. It is unlikely that the first 3 explanations—patients’ self-awareness, reporting bias, or compliance—would be dramatically different in the choice versus the usual care groups. However, it is certainly possible that the act of choosing one’s own therapy might have an impact on the 4th scenario (i.e., that the expectation itself is therapeutic). A component of positive expectation may be confidence that a health care provider will choose the appropriate intervention to heal the patient. Perhaps placing that choice entirely in the hands of the patient reduces the overall effect of the expectation. (As discussed below, patients in our study were not provided with the information, support, and dialog critical to shared decision making). Whereas this line of argument can only be conjectural based on our own research, it is worth further study.

One limitation of our study is that it did not have adequate power to evaluate whether the association between expectation and outcome was different in the choice versus usual care groups. Because an interaction term is needed to evaluate this type of effect modification, and because the interaction term is the product of 2 variables, we would have required 4 times as many subjects to maintain the same power to evaluate this question. Despite dramatically different ?-coefficients for choice versus usual care (0.27 vs 0.80) even the week 12 interaction term was just short of statistical significance.

A second limitation was that the general and specific expectation questions were formatted somewhat differently, which makes a direct comparison of the responses problematic.



IMPLICATIONS/CONCLUSIONS

Given the relatively strong association between expectation and outcome, one implication of this research is that clinicians who wish to estimate their patients’ prognoses may wish to assess general expectations for improvement as part of their initial evaluation of ALBP. This study did not evaluate whether changing a patient’s expectations leads to improved outcomes, but it does suggest that patients with higher expectations for speedy recovery are likely to show more improvement. For example, on average, a patient who rated his general expectation for recovery a 6 would have nearly a 3-point lower Roland score at 5 weeks than a patient with the same functional status who rated his expectation a 9 on the 0–10 scale. This large a difference in the Roland score is clinically significant. In fact, it represents a larger difference in outcome than the difference between patients with and without sciatica.

It has often been assumed that allowing patients to choose the therapy for which they have greater preference confers a therapeutic advantage. [16] Whereas this has been studied to some extent, particularly in depression, no consistent therapeutic advantage has been shown in giving patients their choice of therapy. [17, 18] It may be that the type of choice offered to patients and the amount of support that patients receive in making informed decisions play important roles in determining the impact of the choice itself. It cannot be overemphasized that in our study we did not use a shared decision-making model within which patients are assisted in making choices that are optimized to their particular preferences and are informed by interaction with health care providers. [19, 20] Instead, patients were left on their own to make decisions about which they may, or may not, have been well-informed.

Our study raises several questions about the effect of patient expectations on clinical outcomes. Why is it that patients’ expectations appear to be consistently associated with their clinical outcomes? Do changes in the therapeutic intervention, including giving patients unassisted choice, modify the effect of expectations? Does altering patients’ expectations alter their outcomes? The answers to these questions are relevant to clinical practice and to the design of future trials. Priorities for increasing our understanding of these complex associations include designing validated and uniform instruments for assessing patient expectations and developing optimal study designs. Including questions about patient expectations in future trials would be an inexpensive but potentially valuable way to increase our understanding of these associations and may also prove important in preventing confounding by this poorly understood, but significant, correlate of clinical outcomes.


Funding/Support

The project described was supported by Grant Number 1 R01 AT 00144 from the National Center for Complementary and Alternative Medicine. This work has also been supported in part by a generous endowment from the Bernard Osher Foundation and an unrestricted educational grant from American Specialty Health. Dr. Myers was supported by an Institutional National Research Service Award (T32AT00051-06) from the National Institutes of Health when this research was performed. Dr. Phillips is supported by a Mid-Career Investigator Award from the National Center for Complementary and Alternative Medicine, National Institutes of Health (K24-AT000589). The funding organizations did not provide any input into the design and conduct of the study; collection, management, analysis, and interpretation of the data; or preparation, review or approval of the manuscript.


Conflict of Interest

None disclosed.



References:

  1. Burton KE, Wright V, Richards J.
    Patients’ expectations in relation to outcome of total hip replacement surgery.
    Ann Rheum Dis. 1979;38(5):471–4

  2. Flood AB, Lorence DP, Ding J, McPherson K, Black NA.
    The role of expectations in patients’ reports of post-operative outcomes and improvement following therapy.
    Med Care. 1993;31(11):1043–56

  3. Iversen MD, Daltroy LH, Fossel AH, Katz JN.
    The prognostic importance of patient pre-operative expectations of surgery for lumbar spinal stenosis.
    Patient Educ Couns. 1998;34(2):169–78

  4. Kalauokalani D, Cherkin DC, Sherman KJ, Koepsell TD, Deyo RA.
    Lessons from a trial of acupuncture and massage for low back pain:
    patient expectations and treatment effects.
    Spine. 2001;26(13):1418–24

  5. Leedham B, Meyerowitz BE, Muirhead J, Frist WH.
    Positive expectations predict health after heart transplantation.
    Health Psychol. 1995;14(1):74–9

  6. Linde KWC, Streng A, Weidenhammer W, Wagenpfeil S, Brinkhaus B, Willich SN, Melchart D.
    The impact of patient expectations on outcomes in four randomized controlled trials of
    acupuncture in patients with chronic pain.
    Pain. 2007;128(3):264–71

  7. Lutz GK, Butzlaff ME, Atlas SJ, Keller RB, Singer DE, Deyo RA.
    The relation between expectations and outcomes in surgery for sciatica.
    J Gen Intern Med. 1999;14(12):740–4

  8. Tielsch JM, Steinberg EP, Cassard SD, Schein OD, Javitt JC, Legro MW, et al.
    Preoperative functional expectations and postoperative outcomes among patients undergoing
    first eye cataract surgery.
    Arch Ophthalmol. 1995;113(10):1312–8

  9. Mondloch MV, Cole DC, Frank JW.
    Does how you do depend on how you think you’ll do? A systematic review of the evidence for
    a relation between patients’ recovery expectations and health outcomes.
    CMAJ Can Med Assoc J. 2001;165(2):174–9

  10. Roscoe JA, Morrow GR, Hickok JT, et al.
    The efficacy of acupressure and acustimulation wrist bands for the relief of chemotherapy-induced
    nausea and vomiting. A University of Rochester Cancer Center Community Clinical Oncology
    Program multicenter study.
    Journal of Pain and Symptom Management. 2003;26(2):731–42

  11. Mahomed NN, Liang MH, Cook EF, et al.
    The importance of patient expectations in predicting functional outcomes after total joint arthroplasty.
    J Rheumatol. 2002;29(6):1273–9

  12. Eisenberg DM, Post DE, Davis RB, et al.
    Addition of choice of complementary therapies to usual care for acute low back pain:
    a randomized controlled trial.
    Spine. 2007;32(2):151–8

  13. Patrick DL, Deyo RA, Atlas SJ, Singer DE, Chapin A, Keller RB.
    Assessing health-related quality of life in patients with sciatica.
    Spine. 1995;20(17):1899–908; discussion 1909

  14. Roland M, Morris R.
    A study of the natural history of back pain.
    Part I: development of a reliable and sensitive measure of disability in low-back pain.
    Spine. 1983;8(2):141–4

  15. Roland M, Morris R.
    A study of the natural history of low-back pain.
    Part II: development of guidelines for trials of treatment in primary care.
    Spine. 1983;8(2):145–50

  16. McPherson K, Britton A.
    Preferences and understanding their effects on health.
    Qual Health Care. 2001;10(suppl 1):i61–6

  17. Chilvers C, Dewey M, Fielding K, et al.
    Antidepressant drugs and generic counselling for treatment of major depression in primary care:
    randomised trial with patient preference arms.
    BMJ. 2001;322(7289):772–5

  18. Bedi N, Chilvers C, Churchill R, et al.
    Assessing effectiveness of treatment of depression in primary care.
    Partially randomised preference trial.
    Br J Psychiatry. 2000;177:312–8

  19. Barry MJ, Mulley AG Jr, Fowler FJ, Wennberg JW.
    Watchful waiting vs immediate transurethral resection for symptomatic prostatism.
    The importance of patients’ preferences.
    JAMA. 1988;259(20):3010–7

  20. Kassirer JP.
    Incorporating patients’ preferences into medical decisions.
    N Engl J Med. 1994;330(26):1895–6.

Return to PATIENT EXPECTATIONS OF RELIEF

Since 4–23-2020

                  © 1995–2024 ~ The Chiropractic Resource Organization ~ All Rights Reserved